語系:
繁體中文
English
說明(常見問題)
登入
回首頁
切換:
標籤
|
MARC模式
|
ISBD
Productive Responses to Failure for ...
~
ProQuest Information and Learning Co.
Productive Responses to Failure for Future Learning.
紀錄類型:
書目-語言資料,手稿 : Monograph/item
正題名/作者:
Productive Responses to Failure for Future Learning./
作者:
Lee, Alison.
面頁冊數:
1 online resource (122 pages)
附註:
Source: Dissertation Abstracts International, Volume: 79-01(E), Section: B.
Contained By:
Dissertation Abstracts International79-01B(E).
標題:
Cognitive psychology. -
電子資源:
click for full text (PQDT)
ISBN:
9780355153613
Productive Responses to Failure for Future Learning.
Lee, Alison.
Productive Responses to Failure for Future Learning.
- 1 online resource (122 pages)
Source: Dissertation Abstracts International, Volume: 79-01(E), Section: B.
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Columbia University, 2017.
Includes bibliographical references
For failure experiences to be productive for future performance or learning, students must be both willing to persist in the face of failure, and effective in gleaning information from their errors. While there have been extensive advances in understanding the motivational dispositions that drive resilience and persistence in the face of failure, less has been done to investigate what strategies and learning behaviors students can undertake to make those failure experiences productive. This dissertation investigates what kinds of behaviors expert learners (in the form of graduate students) employ when encountering failure that predict future performance (Study 1), and whether such effective behaviors can be provoked in less sophisticated learners (in the form of high school students) that would subsequently lead to deeper learning (Study 2). Study 1 showed that experiencing and responding to failures in an educational electrical circuit puzzle game prior to formal instruction led to deeper learning, and that one particular strategy, "information-seeking and fixing", was predictive of higher performance. This strategy was decomposed into three metacognitive components: error specification, where the subject made the realization that a knowledge gap or misunderstanding led to the failure; knowledge gap resolution, where the subject sought information to resolve the knowledge gap; and application, where subjects took their newly acquired information to fix their prior error. In Study 2, two types of prompts were added to the educational game: one that provoked students through these metacognitive steps of error specification, information seeking, and fixing, labelled the "Metacognitive Failure Response" (MFR) condition; and a second prompt that provoked students to make a global judgment of knowing, labelled the "Global Awareness" (GA) condition. The results indicated that although there were no significant condition differences between the three groups (MFR, GA, and control condition where participants received no prompt at all), more time spent on the MFR prompt predicted deeper and more robust learning. In contrast, more time spent on the "Global Awareness" prompt did not predict deeper learning, suggesting that individual factors (such as conscientiousness) did not alone account for the benefits of time spent on the MFR prompt on learning. These results suggest that while MFR participants who carefully attended to the metacognitive prompts to specify the source of their errors and seek information experienced learning benefits, not all MFR participants sufficiently attended to the prompts enough to experience learning gains. Altogether, this body of research suggests that using this "error specification, info-seeking, fixing" strategy can be effective for making failure productive, but other instructional techniques beyond system-delivered prompts must be employed for full adoption of this metacognitive response to failure. Implications for teaching students to respond effectively to failure, for games in the classroom, and for design and engineering processes are discussed.
Electronic reproduction.
Ann Arbor, Mich. :
ProQuest,
2018
Mode of access: World Wide Web
ISBN: 9780355153613Subjects--Topical Terms:
556029
Cognitive psychology.
Index Terms--Genre/Form:
554714
Electronic books.
Productive Responses to Failure for Future Learning.
LDR
:04335ntm a2200337Ki 4500
001
918486
005
20181026115416.5
006
m o u
007
cr mn||||a|a||
008
190606s2017 xx obm 000 0 eng d
020
$a
9780355153613
035
$a
(MiAaPQ)AAI10608313
035
$a
(MiAaPQ)columbia:14119
035
$a
AAI10608313
040
$a
MiAaPQ
$b
eng
$c
MiAaPQ
$d
NTU
100
1
$a
Lee, Alison.
$3
884913
245
1 0
$a
Productive Responses to Failure for Future Learning.
264
0
$c
2017
300
$a
1 online resource (122 pages)
336
$a
text
$b
txt
$2
rdacontent
337
$a
computer
$b
c
$2
rdamedia
338
$a
online resource
$b
cr
$2
rdacarrier
500
$a
Source: Dissertation Abstracts International, Volume: 79-01(E), Section: B.
500
$a
Adviser: John B. Black.
502
$a
Thesis (Ph.D.)--Columbia University, 2017.
504
$a
Includes bibliographical references
520
$a
For failure experiences to be productive for future performance or learning, students must be both willing to persist in the face of failure, and effective in gleaning information from their errors. While there have been extensive advances in understanding the motivational dispositions that drive resilience and persistence in the face of failure, less has been done to investigate what strategies and learning behaviors students can undertake to make those failure experiences productive. This dissertation investigates what kinds of behaviors expert learners (in the form of graduate students) employ when encountering failure that predict future performance (Study 1), and whether such effective behaviors can be provoked in less sophisticated learners (in the form of high school students) that would subsequently lead to deeper learning (Study 2). Study 1 showed that experiencing and responding to failures in an educational electrical circuit puzzle game prior to formal instruction led to deeper learning, and that one particular strategy, "information-seeking and fixing", was predictive of higher performance. This strategy was decomposed into three metacognitive components: error specification, where the subject made the realization that a knowledge gap or misunderstanding led to the failure; knowledge gap resolution, where the subject sought information to resolve the knowledge gap; and application, where subjects took their newly acquired information to fix their prior error. In Study 2, two types of prompts were added to the educational game: one that provoked students through these metacognitive steps of error specification, information seeking, and fixing, labelled the "Metacognitive Failure Response" (MFR) condition; and a second prompt that provoked students to make a global judgment of knowing, labelled the "Global Awareness" (GA) condition. The results indicated that although there were no significant condition differences between the three groups (MFR, GA, and control condition where participants received no prompt at all), more time spent on the MFR prompt predicted deeper and more robust learning. In contrast, more time spent on the "Global Awareness" prompt did not predict deeper learning, suggesting that individual factors (such as conscientiousness) did not alone account for the benefits of time spent on the MFR prompt on learning. These results suggest that while MFR participants who carefully attended to the metacognitive prompts to specify the source of their errors and seek information experienced learning benefits, not all MFR participants sufficiently attended to the prompts enough to experience learning gains. Altogether, this body of research suggests that using this "error specification, info-seeking, fixing" strategy can be effective for making failure productive, but other instructional techniques beyond system-delivered prompts must be employed for full adoption of this metacognitive response to failure. Implications for teaching students to respond effectively to failure, for games in the classroom, and for design and engineering processes are discussed.
533
$a
Electronic reproduction.
$b
Ann Arbor, Mich. :
$c
ProQuest,
$d
2018
538
$a
Mode of access: World Wide Web
650
4
$a
Cognitive psychology.
$3
556029
650
4
$a
Psychology.
$3
555998
655
7
$a
Electronic books.
$2
local
$3
554714
690
$a
0633
690
$a
0621
710
2
$a
ProQuest Information and Learning Co.
$3
1178819
710
2
$a
Columbia University.
$b
Cognitive Studies in Education.
$3
1188096
773
0
$t
Dissertation Abstracts International
$g
79-01B(E).
856
4 0
$u
http://pqdd.sinica.edu.tw/twdaoapp/servlet/advanced?query=10608313
$z
click for full text (PQDT)
筆 0 讀者評論
多媒體
評論
新增評論
分享你的心得
Export
取書館別
處理中
...
變更密碼[密碼必須為2種組合(英文和數字)及長度為10碼以上]
登入